Abstract

Attention to a specific target or location in visual space enhances the baseline activity of the cells representing the target or the spatial location. Attention can also be directed based on the expectations. Attention mediated enhanced baseline activity is correlated with improved object recognition. To explore the relation of visual attention with neural baseline activity, cortical sensory processing and the behavioral choice we recorded the activity of single cells in the inferior temporal cortex of monkeys during two different tasks. The tasks were a passive fixation and a two-alternative forced choice categorization of noisy body and object images. We found enhanced neural activity in categorization task compared to the passive fixation task. Both body and object selective cells showed significantly more response enhancement for their preferred category compared to the non-preferred category. No such response enhancement was observed in trials when the monkeys made a wrong choice in the categorization task. Magnitude of the response enhancement was larger for more noisy stimuli. More importantly, in trials with high baseline activity responses of body selective and object selective cells to body images were enhanced and suppressed, respectively. We also found decreased neural response variability in the categorization compared to the passive task. Larger effects were observed at higher noise levels. By measuring choice probability we found that neural firing rate was correlated with monkeys’ choice, particularly in trials with high baseline activity. We suggest that attentional enhancement of IT cells’ baseline firing rate is correlated with improved neural response reliability and category selectivity. These effects are dependent on the cells’ category selectivity, attentional load and the exact time of baseline activity increase.

 

Keywords: object recognition, neural baseline activity, visual attention, decision making

 

 

Table of Contents

Introduction .. .10

  1. The crucial role of “visual object categorization: in everyday life … 10
  2. Where in the brain is category information represented? …………11
  3. Anatomy of inferior temporal cortex . 23
  4. Attention improves categorization performance, especially in difficult condition … .25
  5. Bottom-up vs. top-down attention . 26
  6. What is attention directed to? . …28

    1. Space-based attention .28
    2. Feature-based attention …28
    3. Object-based attention .. .. . 29
  7. Sources and targets of attention in the brain . 31
  8. Attention modulates different response properties .. ..31

    1. Firing rate modulation . 32

      1. Response enhancement. …………. .. .32
      2. Response suppression … 33




  • Baseline enhancement .. 34
  1. Reliability increase . .36
  2. Response sensitivity increase . .37
  3. Response selectivity modulation . 38
  4. Synchronization, oscillation and correlated responses across cell population … .39

Objectives .. .42

Method 43

  1. Subjects . 43
  2. Stereotactic MRI . ..43
  3. Head-post implantation surgery. . ..44
  4. Stimuli . ..46
  5. Tasks … … 47

    1. Passive task … ..47
    2. Active task (two-alternative forced-choice body/object categorization) .47
  6. Training . 50
  7. Eye monitoring . .52
  8. Craniotomy surgery . .52
  9. Recording . .53

    1. Recoded area . ..54
    2. Recording room . ..54
    3. Data acquisition setup . 54
    4. Noise reduction . ..55
    5. Electrode insertion .56
    6. Signal amplification and frequency filtering .57

  10. Data analysis .. …59

    1. Category selectivity index . ..60
    2. High and low baseline trials . … 60
    3. RMI (rate modulation index) .. 61
    4. FF (fano factor) . ..61
    5. FFMI (fano factor modulation index) . 61
    6. CP (choice probability) …62
    7. RMI onset …65
    8. Neural/behavioral score ..66
    9. Peristimulus time histograms (PSTH), normalizing and smoothing .67


 

Results .68

Conclusion ..88

Figures 90

مقالات و پایان نامه ارشد

 

  1. Stimulus set . .. .90
  2. Figure 2. Different noise levels of an exemplar stimulus . 91
  3. Figure 3. Passive task ….92
  4. Figure 4. Active task (two-alternative forced-choice body/object categorization …. 93
  5. Figure 5. Monkeys’ performance in body/object categorization task. .95
  6. Figure 6. The pattern of performance decline as a function of noise level was reverse for bodies and objects ….. 96
  7. Figure 7. Monkeys’ performance in body/object categorization task for subcategories .. ..97
  8. Figure 8 .Performance decline between adjacent signal levels in subcategories of bodies and objects . 98
  9. Figure 9. Behavioral d́ (d́ = Z “hit rate” – Z “false alarm”) in different visual signals … . .. 99
  10. Figure 10. Cumulative d́ in signal level of 90 . …100
  11. Figure 11. Reaction time in different signal conditions in correct and wrong trials .101

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

  1. Figure 12. Reaction time in subcategory level 102
  2. Figure 13. Relation between reaction time and performance in different signal levels . 103
  3. Figure 14. Mean number of microsaccades in different noise levels ..104
  4. Figure 15. Mean number of microsaccades in correct and wrong trials of different signal levels ..105
  5. Figure 16. Reaction time in trials with and without microsaccades in different signal levels .. 106
  6. Figure 17. Normalized mean firing rate of body cells across different visual signals and behavioral conditions . ..107
  7. Figure 18. Normalized mean firing rate of non-body cells across different visual signals and behavioral conditions …..109
  8. Figure 19.Response modulation index (RMI) as a function of task difficulty . .111
  9. Figure 20. Mean response modulation onset across body image signal levels in body cells’ correct trials . .112
  10. Figure 21. Attentional enhancement of IT cells’ body-object discriminability (d’) was observed only in correct trials and degree of enhancement depended on task difficulty .. 114
  11. Figure 22. Mean d’ modulation in correct (blue) and wrong (black) compared to passive condition in body cells … 116
  12. Figure 23. Mean d’ modulation in correct (blue) and wrong (black) compared to passive condition in non-body cells 117
  13. Figure 24. Temporal pattern of baseline firing rate modulation in active compared to passive condition ….118
  14. Figure 25. Temporal pattern of p-values of t-tests measuring significant increase of baseline rate in active compared to passive condition .. 119
  15. Figure 26. Frequency distribution of proportion of HBTs in body (top) and non-body (bottom) cells during active task . 120
  16. Figure 27. Baseline dependent enhancement of body and suppression of non-body cells’ responses to presentation of body images in correct condition . .121
  17. Figure 28. Baseline dependent enhancement of body and suppression of non-body cells’ responses to presentation of body images in wrong condition .. 122
  18. Figure 29. Temporal dynamic of body and non-body cells’ RMI to presentation of body images in correct and wrong conditions for HBTs … 123
  19. Figure 30. Temporal dynamic of body and non-body cells’ RMI to presentation of body images in correct and wrong conditions for LBTs 124
  20. Figure 31. P-values of t-tests measuring significant enhancement of body and suppression of non-body cells’ responses in HBTs as time window to define high baseline activity varied over time .125
  21. Figure 32. P-values of t-tests measuring significantly larger RMI values of body and smaller RMI values of non-body cells’ in HBTs vs. LBTs as time window to define high vs. low baseline activity varied over time . .126
  22. Figure 33. Body cells’ RMI values of high and low baseline trials across body stimulus signal levels . 127
  23. Figure 34. Baseline dependent modulation of body and non-body cells’ responses to presentation of object images . 129
  24. Figure 35. Baseline dependent modulation of body and non-body cells’ responses to presentation of object images . 130
  25. Figure 36. Temporal dynamics of body and non-body cells’ RMI to presentation of object images in correct and wrong conditions for HBTs …131
  26. Figure 37. Temporal dynamics of body and non-body cells’ RMI to presentation of object images in correct and wrong conditions for LBTs 132
  27. Figure 38. Frequency distribution of adjusted RMI values for body and non-body cells in HBTs and LBTs . 133
  28. Figure 39. Rate-matched fano factor modulation index (FFMI) of body and non-body cells to presentation of body images in correct condition for HBTs vs. LBTs .. 134
  29. Figure 40. Rate-matched fano factor modulation (FFMI) of body and non-body cells to presentation of object images in correct conditions for HBTs and LBTs. … 135
  30. Figure 41a. Frequency distribution of normalized d’ modulation difference in LBTs vs. HBTs for body and non-body cells …..136
  31. Figure 41b. The impact of task specific attentional modulation on firing rate depends on cells’ category selectivity …..137
  32. Figure 42. The impact of task specific attentional modulation on firing rate depends on cells’ category selectivity . 138
  33. Figure 43. Comparison of RMI values of correct vs. wrong trials of LBTs and HBTs . 139
  34. Figure 44. Comparison of rate modulation in body and non-body cells population across trials of body images with different baseline spike counts ..140
  35. Figure 45. Comparison of rate modulation in body and non-body cells population across trials of object images with different baseline spike counts . .142
  36. Figure 46. Baseline dependent correlation of neural activity and behavioral choice .. 143
  37. Figure 47. Correlation between CP and cells’ body/object discrimination power …144
  38. Figure 48. CP values of body cells plotted against the HBTs proportion in active task ….145
  39. Figure 49. RMI values of body cells plotted against the HBTs proportion in active task 146
  40. Figure 50. Attentional modulation of baseline and evoked response in 30 low baseline cells 147
  41. Figure 51. Attentional modulation of baseline and evoked response in 30 low baseline cells 148
  42. Figure 52. Percent of HBT in active is plotted vs. percent of HBT in passive for 14 body and 16 non-body cells ..149
  43. Figure 53. RMI of low baseline body and non-body cells in different stimulus and choice conditions … 150
  44. Figure 54. RMI of low baseline body and non-body cells in different stimulus and choice conditions …151
  45. Figure 55. Percent of HBT is active vs. percent of HBT in passive …152
  46. Figure 56. A combination of baseline firing rate and evoked response modulation in active compared with passive conditions affects monkeys’ performance . 153
  47. Figure 57. Polar plots of IT cells activity show that baseline dependent differential response of IT cell subpopulations determines monkey’s choice .. 155

Appendix1: Stimulus set . ….158

Appendix2: List of abbreviations … 164

References … ..166

Introduction

 

The crucial role of “visual object categorization” in everyday life

Our normal life relies on ability of visual object recognition or determining the identity of a seen object. We recognize different familiar or novel objects in everyday life. We do this with no or little effort, despite the fact that these objects may vary in form, color, illumination, view, size or texture from time to time. Based on both behavioral and neural data there are different levels of object recognition. When we see Einstein’s face, first we detect it as a “face” (supraordinate level), perceive as a “human face” (ordinate level) and then “Einstein’s face” (subordinate level). Spector and Kanwisher explored the sequence of processing steps in object recognition by asking human subjects to do three different tasks: object detection, categorization and identification. Accuracy and reaction time were similar for object detection and categorization showing that “as soon as you know it is there, you know what it is” (Spector and Kanwisher, 2005). On the other hand, lower accuracy and longer reaction time was observed for identification compared to categorization, introducing them as different steps of object recognition. Compatible with behavioral data firing patterns of single cells in inferior temporal cortex, a cortical area involved in object recognition, convey the information about categorization and identification with different latencies. Earliest part of the response (~120 ms after stimulus presentation) represents information about categorization while more detailed information about members of category started ~50 ms later (Sugase et al., 1999). Therefore, visual cortex processes information from global to fine in a hierarchical fashion.  It has been suggested that categorization relies on the “presence or absence of features”, whereas identification is based on “configurational judgments”.

“Visual object categorization” or our ability to classify objects by giving meaning to our environment enables us to interact normally and efficiently with objects and events. There are some defined classes of objects in our mind. They usually share some major common properties in their appearance, while at the same time there are lots of differences among their members. For example, trees usually grow from the earth, they have roots, stem and usually green leaves. While they have similar properties, each of the species has a set of specific characteristics. But we call all of them trees, and also easily classify any new member as tree, even if we have not seen something like it before. This fascinating ability of categorization objects is vital for our survival. We know special traits for different object categories. We have learned how to treat and interact with any of them, depending on their characteristics. For example, classifying a rod-shaped moving object as “snake” makes us to run away as fast as possible. We perform this task easily and rapidly under very different conditions and even in noisy environment. Behavioral studies in human have shown that they can recognize animals in a cluttered picture which is presented only for 20ms with reaction times less than 400ms and 95% accuracy (Thorpe et al., 1996; Keysers et al., 2001). Monkeys showed even faster reaction times (Fabre-Thorpe et al., 1998). Monkeys could categorize food and trees with reaction times less than 250ms (Vogels, 1999a). Single cell studies in macaque inferior temporal (IT) cortex have revealed that category response latency is less than 100ms from stimulus onset (Vogels, 1999b; Kiani et al., 2005; Perrett et al., 1982).

 

Where in the brain is category information represented?

Neural mechanisms of and cortical areas involved in visual object categorization are among the hottest areas in field of cognitive neuroscience. Exploring the underlying mechanisms of visual categorization in the activity of single neurons of a special cortical area is based on what Santiago Ramon Cajal proposed by “Neuron Doctrine” over a century ago. He showed that nervous system is not one continuous web but a network of discrete cells. According to “Neuron Doctrine” individual neurons are the basic structural and functional units of the nervous system. This

موضوعات: بدون موضوع  لینک ثابت


فرم در حال بارگذاری ...